Gradual Onset Continuous Performance Test 100 64

  • Journal List
  • Transl Psychiatry
  • v.8; 2018
  • PMC6235862

Transl Psychiatry. 2018; 8: 247.

Continuous performance test impairment in a 22q11.2 microdeletion mouse model: improvement by amphetamine

Simon R. O. Nilsson,1, 2, 3, 4 Christopher J. Heath,5 Samir Takillah,6, 7, 8, 9, 10 Steve Didienne,8 Kim Fejgin,11 Vibeke Nielsen,11 Jacob Nielsen,11 Lisa M. Saksida,1, 2, 12, 13, 14 Jean Mariani,9, 10 Philippe Faure,7 Michael Didriksen,11 Trevor W. Robbins,1, 2 Timothy J. Bussey,1, 2, 12, 13, 14 and Adam C. Mar corresponding author 3, 4

Simon R. O. Nilsson

1Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK

2MRC and Wellcome Trust Behavioural and Clinical Neuroscience Institute, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK

3Neuroscience Institute, New York University Medical Center, New York, NY USA

4Department of Neuroscience and Physiology, School of Medicine, New York University, New York, NY USA

Christopher J. Heath

5School of Life, Health and Chemical Sciences, The Open University, Walton Hall, Milton Keynes, UK

Samir Takillah

6Fatigue and Vigilance team, Neuroscience and Operational Constraints Department, French Armed Forces Biomedical Research Institute (IRBA), Brétigny-sur-Orge, France

7VIFASOM team (EA 7330), Paris Descartes University, Sorbonne Paris Cité, Hôtel Dieu, Paris, France

8Sorbonne Universités, Université Pierre et Marie Curie (UPMC), CNRS, INSERM, U1130, Institut de Biologie Paris Seine (IBPS), UMR 8246 Neuroscience Paris Seine (NPS), Team Neurophysiology and Behavior, Paris, France

9Sorbonne Universités, Université Pierre et Marie Curie (UPMC), CNRS, Institut de Biologie Paris Seine (IBPS), UMR 8256 Biological adaptation and ageing (B2A), Team Brain Development, Repair and Ageing, Paris, France

10APHP Hôpital, DHU Fast, Institut de la Longévité, Ivry-Sur-Seine, France

Steve Didienne

8Sorbonne Universités, Université Pierre et Marie Curie (UPMC), CNRS, INSERM, U1130, Institut de Biologie Paris Seine (IBPS), UMR 8246 Neuroscience Paris Seine (NPS), Team Neurophysiology and Behavior, Paris, France

Kim Fejgin

11H. Lundbeck A/S, Synaptic Transmission, Neuroscience Research DK, Copenhagen, Denmark

Vibeke Nielsen

11H. Lundbeck A/S, Synaptic Transmission, Neuroscience Research DK, Copenhagen, Denmark

Jacob Nielsen

11H. Lundbeck A/S, Synaptic Transmission, Neuroscience Research DK, Copenhagen, Denmark

Lisa M. Saksida

1Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK

2MRC and Wellcome Trust Behavioural and Clinical Neuroscience Institute, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK

12Molecular Medicine Research Group, Robarts Research Institute & Department of Physiology, Western University, London, ON Canada

13Pharmacology, Schulich School of Medicine & Dentistry, Western University, London, ON Canada

14The Brain and Mind Institute, Western University, London, ON Canada

Jean Mariani

9Sorbonne Universités, Université Pierre et Marie Curie (UPMC), CNRS, Institut de Biologie Paris Seine (IBPS), UMR 8256 Biological adaptation and ageing (B2A), Team Brain Development, Repair and Ageing, Paris, France

10APHP Hôpital, DHU Fast, Institut de la Longévité, Ivry-Sur-Seine, France

Philippe Faure

7VIFASOM team (EA 7330), Paris Descartes University, Sorbonne Paris Cité, Hôtel Dieu, Paris, France

Michael Didriksen

11H. Lundbeck A/S, Synaptic Transmission, Neuroscience Research DK, Copenhagen, Denmark

Trevor W. Robbins

1Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK

2MRC and Wellcome Trust Behavioural and Clinical Neuroscience Institute, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK

Timothy J. Bussey

1Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK

2MRC and Wellcome Trust Behavioural and Clinical Neuroscience Institute, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK

12Molecular Medicine Research Group, Robarts Research Institute & Department of Physiology, Western University, London, ON Canada

13Pharmacology, Schulich School of Medicine & Dentistry, Western University, London, ON Canada

14The Brain and Mind Institute, Western University, London, ON Canada

Adam C. Mar

3Neuroscience Institute, New York University Medical Center, New York, NY USA

4Department of Neuroscience and Physiology, School of Medicine, New York University, New York, NY USA

Received 2017 Dec 19; Revised 2018 Aug 21; Accepted 2018 Oct 5.

Supplementary Materials

Supplemental Material (CLEAN)

GUID: 4D95D0F0-68B9-4D24-8A2E-7E072D6BA756

Touchscreen Rodent Continuous Performance Test (rCPT) - Sample Video

GUID: 094AFBB5-8828-48BE-8E33-B4205D145466

Touchscreen Progressive Ratio 4 (PR4) - Sample Video

GUID: 740B91C1-0DFF-4001-866B-E57DD9593C59

Abstract

The 22q11.2 deletion syndrome (22q11.2DS) confers high risk of neurodevelopmental disorders such as schizophrenia and attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. These disorders are associated with attentional impairment, the remediation of which is important for successful therapeutic intervention. We assessed a 22q11.2DS mouse model (Df(h22q11)/+) on a touchscreen rodent continuous performance test (rCPT) of attention and executive function that is analogous to human CPT procedures. Relative to wild-type littermates, Df(h22q11)/+ male mice showed impaired attentional performance as shown by decreased correct response ratio (hit rate) and a reduced ability to discriminate target stimuli from non-target stimuli (discrimination sensitivity, or d'). The Df(h22q11)/+ model exhibited decreased prefrontal cortical-hippocampal oscillatory synchrony within multiple frequency ranges during quiet wakefulness, which may represent a biomarker of cognitive dysfunction. The stimulant amphetamine (0–1.0 mg/kg, i.p.) dose-dependently improved d' in Df(h22q11)/+ mice whereas the highest dose of modafinil (40 mg/kg, i.p.) exacerbated their d' impairment. This is the first report to directly implicate attentional impairment in a 22q11.2DS mouse model, mirroring a key endophenotype of the human disorder. The capacity of the rCPT to detect performance impairments in the 22q11.2DS mouse model, and improvement following psychostimulant-treatment, highlights the utility and translational potential of the Df(h22q11)/+ model and this automated behavioral procedure.

Introduction

A copy number variant (CNV) composed of a hemizygous microdeletion at chromosomal locus 22q11.2 confers large genetic risk for schizophrenia1, attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)2 and autism3. The 22q11.2 microdeletion syndrome (22q11.2DS) and its related neuropsychiatric disorders are associated with executive and attentional impairments4. These deficits are of central interest for translational5 and genetic studies6 aimed at discovering more effective therapeutics.

Attentional and executive dysfunctions are commonly evaluated using computerized continuous performance tests (CPTs)7. Typically, visual target or non-target stimuli are briefly presented at a fixed screen location across a series of continuous, sequential trials. The subject is required to rapidly respond to targets and withhold from responding to non-targets. Non-affected individuals with high genetic load of schizophrenia-related genetic variants8, and 22q11.2 deletion carriers7,9–12, show impaired CPT performance. These impairments predict functional outcome11,13, appear independent of general intelligence9, and are often unaffected by available therapeutics14. Deficits in CPT performance are therefore important targets for therapeutic discovery efforts.

Several mouse models of 22q11.2DS have been generated15–19. Studies investigating the performance of these models across multiple cognitive domains, including associative and spatial learning, flexibility, and memory, have yielded equivocal results19–23. One of the consistent behavioral impairments in these models is an acquisition deficit on a T-maze delayed non-match to position task19,23–25. This deficit has been commonly ascribed to an impairment in working memory and has been linked to prefrontal cortical (PFC)-hippocampal asynchrony within theta and gamma bands20,24. However, the T-maze impairment in the 22q11.2DS mouse model appears transient and delay-independent—inconsistent with a primary deficit in working memory23. PFC-hippocampal synchrony is also associated with executive function and/or attentional processes in other behavioral paradigms26–28.

There is a paucity of studies evaluating attentional function in 22q11.2DS mouse models29. One recent study assessed divided visuospatial attention using the 5-choice serial reaction time task (5-CSRTT) and reported either no effect, or paradoxically improved performance after extended training, in the Df(h22q11)/+ model23. However, to date, there have been no assessments of focused visual attention in 22q11.2DS mouse models. In 22q11.2 deletion carriers, assessments by CPTs and related paradigms demonstrate clinically-relevant impairments on measures of correct response ratio (hit rate) and ability to discriminate target stimuli from non-target stimuli (signal detection sensitivity, or d')7,9–12.

As part of the NEWMEDS initiative (Innovative Medicines Initiative Grant Agreement No. 115008), the current study assessed executive and attentional function in two cohorts of a 22q11.2DS mouse model (Df(h22q11)/+) and wild-type littermates. We evaluated aspects of focused attention and inhibitory control using a touchscreen rodent continuous performance test (rCPT) that has been developed to closely emulate the human paradigm30,31. The rCPT is experimenter-paced and features multiple complex luminance-matched target and non-target stimuli that require detection and discrimination as well as response inhibition31. We hypothesized that the rCPT would be sensitive for identifying attentional impairment in the Df(h22q11)/+ model as measured by d' and/or hit rate. To further characterize the Df(h22q11)/+ mouse model, we investigated PFC-hippocampal coherence which has been proposed as an endophenotype of several neuropsychiatric disorders associated with 22q11.2DS, including schizophrenia32. PFC-hippocampal synchrony has been shown to be disrupted in another 22q11.2DS mouse model (Df(16)A+ /−) while animals are performing a maze task20,24. We probed the robustness and generalizability of this potential endophenotype by evaluating PFC-hippocampal synchrony in a separate cohort of Df(h22q11)/+ mice under quiet-wake "baseline" conditions, independent of potentially confounding influences of prior cognitive training or ongoing behavioral performance. Finally, we assessed the effect of acute systemic modafinil and amphetamine treatments in the (Df(16)A+ /−) model on rCPT performance. The behavioral effects of these drugs have been shown to diverge depending on dose to produce characteristic U-shaped response curves33 with beneficial effects of acute low-dose amphetamine or modafinil frequently being reported on tests of attention and response control in both humans and experimental animals34–36. Moreover, the psychostimulant methylphenidate has been demonstrated to acutely improve discrimination sensitivity, d'37, decrease target omissions and increase hits38 on CPTs in individuals with 22q11.2DS. Based on this evidence we hypothesized that both amphetamine and modafinil would improve d' and/or hit rate of the Df(h22q11)/+ model in the rCPT.

Method

Animals

The generation of Df(h22q11)/+ mice is described elsewhere39. Animals for these experiments were generated by mating wild-type C57BL/6N females with hemizygotic Df(h22q11)/+ males. Young (7–8 weeks), male Df(h22q11)/+ and wild-type littermate offspring were randomly selected and shipped to Cambridge and UPMC for experimentation. Figure 1 depicts the experimental timeline of this study. The behavioral experiments were performed at the University of Cambridge and used two cohorts of male mice housed as previously described23. Sample sizes were selected based on previous rCPT experiments30 and similar touchscreen paradigms40. One cohort of young-adult mice was trained on a progressive ratio (PR) paradigm (aged 9 weeks at start of testing; wild-type N = 16, Df(h22q11)/+N = 15) and subsequently tested on the rCPT (aged 21 weeks at start of testing, wild-type N = 13, Df(h22q11)/+N = 15). Another cohort of older mice (aged 16 months at the start of rCPT testing; wild-type N = 16, Df(h22q11)/+N = 12) was assessed on the rCPT after extensive prior cognitive testing23. Animals were food restricted to about 85% of their free-feeding weight prior to behavioral testing. The electrophysiological studies were performed at UPMC Paris and used 16 male mice aged 3–7 months at testing (wild-type N = 8, Df(h22q11)/+N = 8). All experiments were conducted in accordance with the European Union regulation (directive 2010/63 of 22 September 2010) and the UK Animals (Scientific Procedures) Act 1986.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.  Object name is 41398_2018_295_Fig1_HTML.jpg

Timeline illustrating the experimental treatments and ages of the three cohorts of animals assessed in these experiments.

See Methods for further description of these cohorts

Drugs

Modafinil (Eli Lilly, USA; 0, 0.4, 4.0, 40 mg/kg, i.p, 30 min pretreatment time) was dissolved in vehicle (0.9% sterile saline and 0.5% arabic gum). d-Amphetamine sulphate (Sigma Aldrich, UK; 0, 0.25, 0.5, and 1.0 mg/kg, i.p; 20 min pretreatment time) was dissolved in vehicle (0.9% sterile saline). Dosing protocols were based on previous unpublished and published experiments41–43.

Procedure

Behavioral procedures

See Supplementary Material for video clips of the apparatus and of animals performing the touchscreen rCPT and PR.

Apparatus

The apparatus is described elsewhere40. Briefly, the experiments used touchscreen chambers (Campden Instruments, UK) controlled via commercial (PR; ABET II, Lafaytte Instruments, USA) or in-house software (rCPT; VB.NET 2010, by A.C.M.). The PR task used a 5-aperture mask and the rCPT used a 3-aperture mask as described elsewhere30,41. Animals were trained to approach the touchscreen as detailed previously40.

The rodent continuous performance test

Training—stage 1 (white-square)

The rCPT training procedure is described in detail elsewhere30,31. Briefly, each trial began with a 2 s inter-stimulus interval (ISI) prior to stimulus presentation. To discourage superfluous responding to the screen, the ISI restarted if the subject touched the stimulus window during the ISI ('ISI touch'). After the ISI, a white-square stimulus was presented for a 10 s stimulus duration (SD). If the animal touched the stimulus window within the 'limited hold' (LH) period after stimulus onset (LH:10.5 s), a reward (20 μl strawberry milkshake) was delivered coupled with white-noise (1 s) and magazine light illumination. Following the LH period (non-rewarded trials) or reward collection (rewarded trials), the next ISI was initiated. Trials were presented continuously until the session/phase criterion of 60 rewards was reached (one session for all animals).

Training—stage 2 (1-stimuli)

The correct stimulus (CS+: vertical or horizontal lines, counterbalanced across genotypes) was presented for a 5 s SD (LH:5.5 s). A 5 s delay to allow for reward consumption was added following reward collection. Other parameters remained identical to stage 1. All animals achieved criterion in a single session.

Training—stage 3 (2-stimuli)

On each trial, the mouse was presented with either the CS+ or a novel incorrect stimulus (CS−). The CS+ was identical to stage 2 while the CS− was a 'snowflake' stimulus30. The SD was reduced to 2.5 s (LH:2.5 s), the ISI was increased to 5 s and the CS+ probability was 50%. After a response to the CS−, a correction trial was implemented where the CS− was presented again following the ISI. Correction trials were presented until the animal successfully omitted a response to the CS−. The session ended after 100 correct responses or 45 min, whichever occurred first. Other parameters remained the same as in stage 2. The animals progressed as a group to the baseline rCPT procedure after 5 sessions on stage 3. All animals were performing at d' greater than 0.6 criterion30.

Baseline rCPT (5-stimuli)

On each trial, animals were presented with one of five stimuli: four non-targets and the stage 3 target30. Other parameters remained identical to stage 3. Animals in the younger cohort were assessed for 6 sessions prior to acute, systemic treatment with modafinil and then amphetamine, using randomized Latin-square designs. The older cohort was assessed on baseline rCPT for 2 sessions followed by a series of probe tests (See Supplementary Material).

Progressive ratio

As motivational capacity can influence cognitive task performance, we also assessed Df(h22q11)/+ mice in a progressive ratio (PR) task. Animals in the younger cohort were tested in a touchscreen PR task designed to assess motivation through response requirements that increase according to linear ramp schedules (PR4-PR16) which is described elsewhere41.

Electrophysiology

Surgery

Mice were anesthetized with ketamine/xylazine and placed in a stereotaxic frame. Anesthesia was maintained with 3% isoflurane. Bipolar stainless steel electrodes were implanted bilaterally at coordinates relative to bregma in the infralimbic/prelimbic area of the PFC (dorsal-ventral: −1.55, anterior-posterior: +1.6, medial-lateral: ±0.5 mm) and CA1 region of the dorsal hippocampus (dorsal-ventral: −1.20, anterior-posterior: −1.94, medial-lateral: ± 1.2 mm). Monopolar ground electrodes were laid over the cortical layer of the cerebellum (anterior-posterior: + 6.24, medial-lateral: ± 1.0 mm) and olfactory bulb (anterior-posterior: + 4.2, medial-lateral: ±0.5 mm). Electrodes were fixed to the skull with dental acrylic and connected to an electrode interface board (8 channel headstage EIB-8; Neuralynx, USA). Antiseptic (Povidone-iodine) and local anesthetic (lidocaine) solutions were applied post-surgery. Animals were permitted to recover until regaining pre-surgery body weight.

Signal recording

Recordings were done as previously described44. Briefly, recordings were done in animals using chambers that limited, but did not restrain movement, and were electrically and acoustically insulated and isolated from odors and the experimenters. A cold light (100lux) was placed 20 cm in front of the animal. This environment was used to minimize the known modulatory effects of spontaneous motor activity on hippocampal local field potentials (LFP)45. Animals were gradually acclimated to the recording set-up and procedure. Wild-type and Df(h22q11)/+ mice were assessed simultaneously (4 mice per genotype) using a Latin-square design and recordings were made at the same time each day to minimize circadian LFP effects. Baseline LFP recordings were obtained over 60 min using a Digital Lynx SX (Neuralynx) and were acquired with a cheetah32 data acquisition system (Neuralynx). No attempts to escape or notable stress reactions were observed (i.e. defecation, urination, freezing) during the recording sessions.

Signal analysis

Data were analyzed using Matlab (MathWorks®, USA) built-in functions and the Chronox toolbox46. LFPs were (i) acquired at 1000 Hz and offline band-pass filtered at 0.1–100 Hz with zero-phase shift filter function (zero-phase digital filtering filtfilt function), and (ii) de-rendered using local linear regression (locdetrend function from the Chronux toolbox:46 window-size 1 s, overlap 0.5 s) to remove slow drifts, and (iii) notch-filtered (iirnotch function) with notch located at 50 Hz to remove possible power line noise. The LFP signal was expressed in z-score units. The z-score normalization used the mean and the standard deviation from baseline (entire rest session) of each electrode. Power spectral density (PSD) of LFP data was calculated using the multitaper spectrogram method from the Chronux toolbox with time-bandwidth product of 5 and 10 slepian sequences of orthogonal data tapers (window-size 5 s, 2 s overlap). PSD was averaged over two similar brain regions (right and left hemisphere) for each frequency and time-bin. The multitaper coherogram method was used to calculate coherence (normalized spectral covariance) between the LFP from two structures with time-bandwidth product of 30 and 60 slepian sequences of orthogonal data tapers using a 30 s window-size without overlap. The signal was bandpass-filtered to extract theta oscillations by applying a 5–10 Hz finite impulse response bandpass with zero-phase shift filter function (filtfilt function).

Statistical analysis

Behavior

rCPT hit rate was calculated as the ratio of target responses to target presentations. False alarm rate was calculated as the ratio of non-target responses to non-target presentations. The performance was evaluated using the signal detection measures of discrimination sensitivity (d') and response criterion (c)47. The discrimination sensitivity index (d') assesses the subject's capacity to distinguish the target from the non-target stimuli. The response criterion index (c) assesses the subject's propensity or willingness to respond to any stimulus (e.g., target or non-target). Discrimination sensitivity d' was calculated as48

d = z h i t r a t e - z f a l s e a l a r m r a t e

1

with higher values denoting better ability to discriminate between target and non-target stimuli. Response criterion c was calculated as48

c = - 0.5 z h i t r a t e + z f a l s e a l a r m r a t e

2

with higher values denoting decreased responding to both target and non-target stimuli. ISI touch rate was calculated as the number of touches to the response window during the ISI divided by the total ISI time in minutes. Incorrect and correct response latency and reward latency were also collected. Sessions were further split into 50-trial bins and dependent variables were calculated within each bin. The measures in the PR test were break-point (defined as the number of stimulus responses made in the last successfully completed trial in a session), total touches, total trials, time-out time, and 'blank' touches (defined as responses to the four never-illuminated response locations) per minute41. The experimenter was not blinded to the genotypes/drug-treatments. However, all behavioral data acquisition and analysis were fully automated with no experimenter involvement. Drug-free rCPT and PR data were analyzed by mixed-model ANOVAs with genotype as the between-subjects factor and session, SD, ISI, target probability or stimulus contrast as within-subjects factors. To analyze our a priori hypothesis that the deficits in rCPT performance observed in Df(h22q11)/+ mice could be ameliorated by modafinil or amphetamine, the pharmacological data were analyzed specifically in Df(h22q11)/+ mice using one-way ANOVAs with dose as independent within-subjects factor. To assess the overall effect of these compounds across all animals, the data were also analyzed across both genotypes using mixed-model ANOVAs with genotype as a between-subjects factor and dose as a within-subjects factor. Dose-response patterns were also tested for linear and U-shaped (quadratic) effects31. Significant interactions and dose-response patterns were followed by simple main effect comparisons using one-way ANOVA.

Electrophysiology

Three bands of the PSD were analyzed for each structure: 0.1–3 Hz (delta), 6–12 Hz (theta) and 30–80 Hz (gamma). All datasets were tested for normality using Shapiro-Wilk tests. For multiple comparisons of normally distributed data we used mixed-model ANOVAs (e.g., with frequency bands and genotype as factors). For data with non-Gaussian distributions, we used non-parametric Friedman tests. Post-hoc tests (independent-samples t-tests or Wilcoxon rank-sum test) were performed to compare genotype PSD and coherence estimates to identify frequency bands differing in spectral analysis. The stepwise Holm-Bonferroni (H-B) algorithm was used to correct for family-wise error rate (i.e., potential interference during multiple comparisons) by ordering p-values and adjusting significance level α. Standard error (SEM) intervals were calculated through a jackknife method46.

Results

The rodent continuous performance test

See Supplementary Tables S1–S5 for detailed statistical analysis. rCPT performance of young (aged 21 weeks at the start of testing) Df(h22q11)/+ and wild-type mice is presented in Fig. 2a–d. Training stages 1 and 2, during which no non-target stimuli were presented, did not reveal any effects of genotype (Table S1). When a single, non-target stimulus was introduced, the Df(h22q11)/+ mouse exhibited a near-significant decrease in discrimination sensitivity (d') (Fig. 2a; F1,26 = 4.203, p = 0.051), a significantly decreased hit rate (Fig. 2b; F1,26 = 9.552, p = 0.005) and an increased response criterion (c) (Fig. 2a; F1,26 = 6.971, p = 0.014) relative to wild-type littermate controls. On the baseline 5-stimulus rCPT, the Df(h22q11)/+ model showed decreased d' (Fig. 2c; F1,26 = 5.724, p = 0.030) and decreased hit rate (Fig. 2d; F1,26 = 4.578, p = 0.042) compared to wild-type littermate controls. Time-bin analysis showed that Df(h22q11)/+ mice exhibited impairments throughout the session (data not shown).

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.  Object name is 41398_2018_295_Fig2_HTML.jpg

Performance of Df(h22q11)/ + and wild-type littermates on the 2-stimulus training stage 3 and the baseline 5-stimulus rCPT.

Performance of Df(h22q11)/ + and wild-type littermates on the 2-stimulus training stage 3 and the baseline 5-stimulus rCPT). Data are presented as means ± SEM. Discrimination sensitivity (d') is an index of the subject's ability to distinguish target from non-target stimuli, while response criterion (c) describes the subject's propensity to respond to any stimulus. a 2-stimulus: d' and c. Df(h22q11)/+ mice had increased response criterion c. Df(h22q11)/+ mice showed a non-significant decrease in d' (p = 0.051) relative to littermate controls. b 2-stimulus: hit rate and false alarm rate. Df(h22q11)/+ mice had decreased hit rates relative to controls. There was no effect of genotype on false alarm rate. c 5-stimulus rCPT: d' and c. Df(h22q11)/+ mice had decreased d' relative to controls. There was no effect of genotype on response criterion. d 5-stimulus rCPT: hit rate and false alarm rate. Df(h22q11)/+ mice showed decreased hit rate relative to controls. There was no effect of genotype on false alarm rate. Asterisk denotes significant effect of genotype (*p < 0.05)

A second older cohort of Df(h22q11)/+mice (aged 70 weeks at start of testing), with extensive previous cognitive testing experience, also showed decreased target hit rates when challenged with shorter stimulus durations (Supplementary Fig. S1a–b; genotype × SD: F5,130 = 4.795, p < 0.0001) and increased response criterion c when challenged with longer ISI times (Supplementary Fig. S2c–d; genotype × ISI: F2,50 = 3.221, p = 0.048). See Supplementary Material and Results from this cohort of Df(h22q11)/+ mice when tested on a range of different probe tests.

Electrophysiological recordings

PFC-hippocampal coherence data are presented in Fig. 3. See Supplementary Figures S2–S4 for additional analyses. Representative PFC and hippocampal LFP traces are shown in Fig. 3a. PFC-hippocampal coherence was reduced in Df(h22q11)/+ mice (Fig. 3c; genotype: F1,36 = 16.190, p < 0.001). Post-hoc analyses showed PFC-hippocampal coherence reductions in the delta (p = 0.030, t-test, H-B corrected) theta (p = 0.0027, t-test, H-B corrected) and gamma (p = 0.0035, Wilcoxon rank-sum, H-B corrected) bands of Df(h22q11)/+ mice. Genotype did not affect LFP frequency contents in the PFC (Fig. 3b, left; χ2 = 0.09, p = 0.762, Friedman ANOVA) or hippocampus (Fig. 3b, middle; F1,42 = 7.04, p = 0.601, two-way ANOVA).

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.  Object name is 41398_2018_295_Fig3_HTML.jpg

PFC and hippocampal synchrony and power spectra of Df(h22.q11)/+and wild-type littermates.

Data are presented as mean ± SEM. Shaded areas represent SEM. a Left: Representative traces of the LFP recorded from the same animal simultaneously in the PFC and dorsal hippocampus during steady state conditions. Raw traces are plotted in gray and theta-filtered traces are overlaid in black. Right: Schematic diagram showing locations of LFP recording. b Averaged power spectra for each structure (PFC: left, HPC: middle) and average PFC-hippocampal coherence in 0.1–25 Hz range (right) in Df(h22.q11)/+ and wild-type littermates. c Average PFC-hippocampal coherence in 0.1–100 Hz range (same as b). Asterisk denotes significant effect of genotype (*p < 0.05, **p < 0.005, ***p < 0.0005)

Effects of pharmacological interventions on continuous performance

Modafinil in the Df(h22q11)/+ model

The effect of modafinil in the rCPT is presented in Fig. 4a, b and Table 1. In Df(h22q11)/+ mice, modafinil decreased discrimination sensitivity (d') and incorrect response latency. For d' (Fig. 4a), there was a significant linear effect of dose (F1,14 = 4.947, p = 0.043), with modafinil dose-dependently decreasing d'. The highest 40 mg/kg dose did not significantly reduce d' relative to vehicle (p = 0.075) but significantly reduced d' relative to the 0.4 mg/kg dose (p = 0.021). On incorrect response latency (Table 1), there was a significant main effect of dose (F3,42 = 3.780, p = 0.017) and a significant linear effect of dose (F1,14 = 7.184, p = 0.018), with modafinil dose-dependently reducing incorrect response latency. The 40 mg/kg dose decreased incorrect response latency relative to vehicle (p < 0.0001) and the 4.0 mg/kg dose (p < 0.019).

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.  Object name is 41398_2018_295_Fig4_HTML.jpg

Performance of Df(h22q11)/ + and wild-type littermates on the 5-stimulus rCPT when treated with acute systemic modafinil and amphetamine.

Performance of Df(h22q11)/ + and wild-type littermates on the 5-stimulus rCPT when treated with acute systemic modafinil and amphetamine). Data are presented as means ± SEM. Discrimination sensitivity (d') is an index of the subject's ability to distinguish target from non-target stimuli, while response criterion (c) describes the subject's propensity to respond to any stimulus. a Modafinil: d' and c. Modafinil caused a dose-linear decrease in d' in the Df(h22q11)/+ model. There was no effect of modafinil on response criterion c. b Modafinil: hit rate and false alarm rate. Modafinil had no significant effects on hit rate or false alarm rate. c Amphetamine: d' and c. Amphetamine caused a dose-linear increase in d' in the Df(h22q11)/+ model. 1.0 mg/kg amphetamine tended to increase c in control animals only. d Amphetamine: hit rate and false alarm rate. Amphetamine reduced hit rate in control animals only at the 1.0 mg/kg dose. Amphetamine caused a genotype-independent reduction in hit rate at the 1.0 mg/kg dose. Pink shading denotes significant dose-linear effect that were selective to the Df(h22q11)/+ model (Ψ =p < 0.05). Asterisk denotes significant main effect of genotype (* =p < 0.05). Gray shading and hash denote significant genotype-independent dose differences (# =p < 0.05). Grey shading and lambda denote significant dose differences in Df(h22q11)/+ model (λ =p < 0.05). 'V' denotes drug vehicle condition (modafinil: saline + 0.5% arabic gum, amphetamine: saline)

Table 1

Mean response latencies and ISI touch rate of Df(h22q11) + mice and wild-types littermate controls when treated with acute systemic modafinil and acute systemic amphetamine in the rCPT

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.  Object name is 41398_2018_295_Tab1_HTML.jpg

On false alarm rate (Fig. 4b), there was a significant U-shaped dose-response (F1,14 = 5.508, p = 0.034). However, post-hoc analyses comparing each dose were not significant (p's ≥ 0.075). On ISI touch rate (Table 1), there was a U-shaped dose-response (F3,42 = 5.712, p = 0.031). The 40 mg/kg dose increased ISI touch rate relative to vehicle (p = 0.050) and the 0.04 mg/kg dose (p = 0.005).

Modafinil in both wild-type and Df(h22q11)/ + mice

When the data were speculatively analyzed across both genotypes, we observed a significant impairment in discrimination sensitivity (d') in Df(h22q11)/+ relative to wild-type mice (Fig. 4a; F1,26 = 6.781, p = 0.015). There were no effects of modafinil on d' (Fig. 4a). On incorrect response latency, there was a genotype × dose interaction (Table 1; genotype × dose: F3,78 = 3.263, p = 0.026). In Df(h22q11)/+ mice, the highest 40 mg/kg dose reduced incorrect response latency relative to vehicle (p < 0.0001). This reduction was not present in wild-type animals (p = 0.920).

Modafinil exerted U-shaped dose-response effects on false alarm rate across both genotypes (Fig. 4b; dose: F3,78 = 2.964, p = 0.037, quadratic effect: F1,26 = 5.335, p = 0.029). The 40 mg/kg dose significantly reduced false alarm rate relative to the 4 mg/kg dose (p = 0.028). Modafinil also produced U-shaped dose-responses in both genotypes on ISI touch rate (Table 1; dose: F3,78 = 8.128, p < 0.0001, quadratic effect: F1,26 = 6.708, p = 0.016). Relative to vehicle, the 0.4 mg/kg dose reduced ISI touch rate (p = 0.034) while the 40 mg/kg dose increased ISI touch rate (p = 0.015).

Amphetamine in the 22q11.2 model

In Df(h22q11)/+ mice, amphetamine increased discrimination sensitivity (d'). On d' (Fig. 4c), there was a significant linear effect of dose (F1,14 = 6.683, p = 0.022) with amphetamine dose-dependently improving d'. Relative to vehicle, 1.0 mg/kg amphetamine increased d' (p = 0.018).

Amphetamine in both wild-type and Df(h22q11)/ + mice

When the data were analyzed across both genotypes, the performance-enhancing effect of amphetamine on discrimination sensitivity (d') was not significant (Fig. 4c; see Supplementary Table S5 for statistical analyses). However, a significant dose-linear response-reducing effects of amphetamine on ISI touch rate was observed across both genotypes (Table 1; dose: F3,78 = 3.793, p = 0.014, linear effect: F1,26 = 8.551, p = 0.007). Relative to vehicle, 1 mg/kg of amphetamine decreased ISI touch rate (p = 0.006).

There was also a genotype × dose interaction on hit rate (Fig. 4d; F3,78 = 3.037, p = 0.034). In wild-type animals, the highest 1.0 mg/kg dose caused a reduction in hit rate relative to the 0.5 mg/kg dose (p = 0.046). This reduction was not present in Df(h22q11)/+ mice (p = 0.848). There was a dose-linear effect of amphetamine on false alarm rate (Fig. 4d; F1,26 = 4.629, p = 0.041). Relative to vehicle, 1.0 mg/kg amphetamine decreased false alarm rates across both genotypes (p = 0.025). There was also a main effect of dose on response criterion (c) (Fig. 4c; F3,78 = 2.779, p = 0.047). However post-hoc analyses comparing each dose were not significant (p's ≥ 0.058).

Progressive ratio

PR performance for Df(h22q11)/+ mice and wild-type littermates is presented in Fig. 5. There was no effect of genotype on break-point (genotype: F1,29 = 0.882, p = 0.355, genotype × PR schedule: F3,87 = 0.603, p = 0.615) or any other performance measurement (p ≥ 0.096; data not shown).

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.  Object name is 41398_2018_295_Fig5_HTML.jpg

Performance of Df(h22q11)/+ and wild-types littermates on progressive ratio schedules.

Data are presented as means ± SEM. There was no effect of genotype on break-point

Discussion

The present study revealed that the 22q11.2DS mouse model (Df(h22q11)/+) exhibits neuropsychiatric disease-relevant impairments in focused visual attention. Similar rCPT impairments were observed across two cohorts that differed in age and experimental training history. These impairments in the 22q11.2DS model occurred in the absence of motivational, motoric, visual or other cognitive changes23, indicative of highly selective deficits in visual attentional control. Parallel with these behavioral abnormalities, the 22q11.2DS mouse model showed reduced PFC-hippocampal oscillatory synchrony in gamma, delta, and theta bands, without altered basal oscillatory activity within each region. The model deficits in discrimination sensitivity (d') were dose-dependently improved by acute, low-dose amphetamine and, contrary to our predictions, dose-dependently impaired by acute modafinil treatment. Taken together, these data indicate a robust, selective, translationally-relevant attentional impairment in a 22q11.2DS mouse model that closely mirrors a key cognitive endophenotypic marker of 22q11.2DS and related psychiatric disorders.

Attentional dysfunction in the Df(h22q11)/+ mouse

Attentional deficits are central to 22q11.2DS symptomatology49. Individuals with 22q11.2DS show CPT impairments7,10,12,50 and 30–40% of 22q11.2 deletion carriers are diagnosed with schizophrenia1 or ADHD3, disorders where CPT impairments represent core endophenotypes51,52. Individuals with 22q11.2DS, as well as with schizophrenia and ADHD, typically show decreased discrimination sensitivity (d')7,9.

Deficits were observed in two separate cohorts of Df(h22q11)/+ mice that varied in both age (5 vs. 16 months at the start of testing) and previous cognitive testing experience. Although the impairments in the older cohort were somewhat less pronounced—found only under task conditions which taxed attentional load (i.e., reduced stimulus duration or increased inter-stimulus interval)—it is notable that both cohorts showed selective impairments in target hit rate. The wide age-range and relative robustness of the Df(h22q11)/+ deficits further reinforce the translational relevance of the model on the rCPT paradigm. Longitudinal studies have shown that CPT attentional impairments can persist in 22q11.2DS7, schizophrenia53, and ADHD54 and might represent a key endophenotypic marker of these disorders29,55,56.

The Df(h22q11)/+ mice appear to have cognitive deficits on the rCPT that are specific to attentional processes. We also demonstrate that Df(h22q11)/+ mice have intact motivation in a touchscreen progressive ratio task. No persistent impairment was previously observed in the model using a large cognitive testing battery23, and studies of cognition in alternative 22q11.2DS mouse models have generally yielded mixed results (see Table 1 in ref. 23). This is in apparent disparity with the clinical syndrome which has been associated with widespread and often non-selective cognitive impairment57. It is possible that the selective attentional impairment of the 22q11.2DS mouse model might require interaction with certain environmental risk factors to induce a more profound phenotype58,59. There are currently no reports assessing the effects of environmental manipulations on cognitive function in 22q11.2DS mouse models.

The mechanisms underlying impaired attention in 22q11.2DS are unknown49. Imaging studies of individuals with 22q11.2.DS show abnormalities within brain networks supporting attention, including structural60,61 and connectivity deficits61,62 within and between the striatum, PFC, cingulate, and temporal cortices. Structural deficits in the dorsolateral PFC and cingulate cortices of 22q11.2 deletion carriers correlate with CPT impairments10. Immuno-, electrophysiological- and imaging assays revealed PFC abnormalities in other 22q11.2DS models63–65. Such abnormalities include PFC-hippocampal theta and gamma coherence disruptions that correlate with the slower learning in a T-maze task20,24.

We observed similar coherence abnormalities in the current study using a 22q11.2DS model on a different background strain and employing a different recording environment (under immobile conditions). The results validate the findings of previous reports20,24, and additionally demonstrate that the presence of PFC-HPC asynchrony in the 22q11.2DS model is unrelated to cognitive training and ongoing behavioral performance. PFC-hippocampal synchrony aberrations may represent disrupted longer-range information integration/coordination in schizophrenia32, and the presence of similar disruptions in Df(h22q11)/+ mice may support its validity for evaluating genetic causes for psychopathology.

Pharmacological effects

Modafinil and amphetamine can improve cognitive functions in humans66–68 and experimental animals42,69–72. The drugs nevertheless have both common and distinct biochemical effects68. The vigilance-promoting effects of amphetamine have been attributed primarily to increased dopamine/noradrenaline activity in prefrontal systems33, whereas modafinil has additional actions on serotonin, hypocretin/orexin, glutamate, histamine and acetylcholine functions68. Our data indicate that acute treatment with these drugs exerts modest but bidirectional effects on attentional performance in Df(h22q11)/+ mice.

Modafinil

We observed a small but significant reduction in discrimination sensitivity (d') following acute modafinil treatment in the Df(h22q11)/+ model. This linear dose-dependent reduction in d' was concomitant with speeding of incorrect response latency. An impairing effect of 40 mg/kg modafinil on d' in the Df(h22q11)/+ model is similar to the observed higher-dose effects (64 mg/kg) on the rCPT in the MAM-E17 rat model of schizophrenia31. It is also consistent with the higher-dose effects of modafinil (64–100 mg/kg) observed in other tests of attention and/or inhibitory control, including decreased accuracy73 and increased premature responding in the 5-CSRTT73,74 and impaired Go-accuracy in the stop-signal reaction time task43. Low-to-moderate doses of modafinil have nevertheless been shown to improve stop-signal reaction time (10 mg/kg43) and CPT d' in healthy rats (8 mg/kg)31 and fronto-striatal dependent cognition, including attention, in humans (100–200mg75–77). Unlike amphetamine, modafinil-induced improvements have generally been ascribed to enhanced inhibitory control processes that are detected in low-performing sub-groups and/or when task conditions are implemented that further challenge the ability to withhold responses31,77. Amphetamine has higher potency at dopamine/noradrenaline transporters than modafinil;78 suggesting that modafinil's impairing effects involve additional transmitter systems. For example, modafinil, but not low-dose amphetamine, increases PFC 5-HT levels79. Such 5-HT increases may produce detrimental effects on impulsive-like behavior and attention when concurrent with elevations in striatal dopaminergic tone80,81, as displayed by the Df(h22q11)/+ model39.

The lack of facilitatory effects from lower-dose modafinil in the present study might be explained by the fact that, at the current task parameters, animals had low false alarm rates (~ 0.1) and thus, floor-effects could preclude detection of cognitive-enhancing effects of modafinil. By contrast, reduced baselines for false alarms would not prevent psychostimulants such as amphetamine and methylphenidate - which also enhance hit rates38 - from exerting performance-enhancing effects. Task parameters that challenge inhibitory control processes (e.g., variable ISIs, SDs, CS+ probabilities, flanking distractors) may be more amenable for uncovering pro-cognitive effects from low-dose modafinil in the rCPT, as was observed in the rat using variable SDs and ISIs31.

Amphetamine

We observed a significant, dose-dependent, enhancement of discrimination sensitivity (d') in Df(h22q11)/+ animals treated with amphetamine. This improvement is in translational agreement with data showing that acute administration of the mechanistically-similar stimulant, methylphenidate, can improve d' in children and adolescents with 22q11.2DS on a visual CPT paradigm37. The data are consistent with reports of low-dose stimulants improving CPT d' in ADHD82 and the performances of individuals with schizophrenia on CPT-like tasks83,84. It is also in line with rodent studies in which low-dose amphetamine (0.25–0.5 mg/kg) or methylphenidate (2.0 mg/kg) improves attentional accuracy on serial reaction time tasks in low-attentive animals or animals challenged with short SDs42,72,85.

The amphetamine-induced improvements in d' in Df(h22q11)/+ mice are due to the combined influence of increasing target hit rate and decreasing non-target false alarm rate, with neither measure showing significant changes on their own. This enhancement in d' was accompanied by a significant reduction in the rate of extraneous touches within the response window on the screen during the ISI (in the absence of any stimuli). Together, the effects of higher discrimination sensitivity (d') and reduced extraneous responding indicate that amphetamine dose-dependently enhances global task performance 'efficiency'. This may reflect a unitary enhancement of attentional control or may be the result of improvements across several distinct cognitive dimensions, including attentional processing (i.e., increases in hit rate and d') and hyperactivity/impulsivity (i.e., decreases in false alarm and ISI touch rate)86. Further work investigating the cognitive mechanisms underlying the performance-enhancing effects of amphetamine is warranted.

The cognitive-enhancing effects of low-dose psychostimulant treatment in the Df(h22q11)/+ model may be produced by preferential activity within the PFC, where concerted actions at noradrenaline transporters and D12 receptors cause downstream glutamatergic and GABAergic events that increase neuronal tuning to behaviorally relevant stimuli33,87. Higher doses, however, have qualitatively different effects from lower doses and consequent deficits in signal processing33,87. Thus, in contrast to the clear cognitive-enhancing effects of amphetamine on rCPT performance in Df(h22q11)/+ mice, the highest dose (1.0 mg/kg) of amphetamine exerted response-suppressant effects in wild-type animals. This effect was seen as a decrease across all response rates, including hit rate, false alarm rate and the rate of ISI responses. There was an associated increase in response criterion without changes in d'. Similar suppressant effects of amphetamine have been observed in healthy well-trained rodents in the 5-CSRTT using comparable doses (≥ 0.8 mg/kg)69,88,89. The distinct effects of 1 mg/kg amphetamine on rCPT performance between Df(h22q11)/+ and wild-type mice (i.e., improvement rather than impairment) might be explained by the model's hemizygosity for catecholamine-O-methyl transferase (COMT). COMT is involved in dopamine degradation primarily in regions with low expression of dopamine transporters, including the PFC90. In healthy rodents, higher-dose (1–1.5 mg/kg) amphetamine has potent effects on striatal relative to PFC dopamine levels91,92 which can have mild stimulant effects93,94 and alters motivational processes95. Decreased COMT dosage in the 22q11.2DS model could increase the ratio of PFC:striatum dopamine transmission following amphetamine treatment, resulting in increased prefrontal, task-specific, cognitive control and in fewer striatally-mediated motoric and/or motivationally-related side effects33. Evidence for such an altered PFC:striatum dopamine ratio has been observed in COMT+/- mice after amphetamine (2.5 mg/kg) treatment, resulting in higher PFC:striatal dopamine turnover compared to wild-type controls96. COMT hemizygosity might additionally reduce degradation of amphetamine-induced norepinephrine release97,98 which, within the demanding rCPT paradigm, might also contribute to the relative absence of a response-suppressant effect of 1.0 mg/kg amphetamine in the Df(h22q11)/+ mouse.

The contrasting effects of amphetamine and modafinil on discrimination sensitivity (d') in the rCPT might be related to differences in the chosen doses within the employed dose ranges, and/or suggest key differences in the relevant cognition-enhancing mechanisms. It would also be valuable to investigate the effects of sub-chronic/chronic dosing of these compounds to evaluate the stability of the pharmacological effects on task performance. Regardless, it is notable that both compounds influence the d' measure, an index widely linked to attentional performance in the human CPT paradigm, selectively in Df(h22q11)/+ mice. The two drugs have been previously shown to exert neurochemical and electrophysiological modulatory effects within and between the PFC and hippocampus33,99. One limitation of the present study is that we did not examine drug effects on PFC-hippocampal coherence. It would be interesting to examine specific drug effects on PFC-hippocampal coherence in the Df(h22q11)/+ model—both independent of behavioral testing, and whilst systematically varying the cognitive demand of the rCPT during various dose regiments—to establish the potential role of electrophysiological correlates in our observed differences in attentional function.

Conclusions

We demonstrate that a 22q11.2DS mouse model has selective impairments on a translationally relevant rCPT test of attention. These impairments are dose-dependently ameliorated by acute amphetamine treatment. These data closely parallel reported CPT impairments in 22q11.2 deletion carriers that are ameliorated by the psychostimulant, methylphenidate. The observed behavioral impairments were paralleled by PFC-hippocampal coherence disruptions within delta, theta and gamma bands during non-task conditions. This is the first report of attentional impairment in a 22q11.2DS model; we have demonstrated a translational utility of the Df(h22q11)/+ mouse in a fully automated and high-throughput procedure that permits large-scale and simultaneous cognitive assessment of multiple animals. In the context of the relatively limited pathology-like phenotypes that have been detected in the Df(h22q11)/+ mouse using alternative cognitive paradigms23, the rCPT may be a useful translational tool with enhanced sensitivity for detecting dysfunctions in rodent models.

Electronic supplementary material

Acknowledgements

The research leading to these results has received support from the Innovative Medicine Initiative Joint Undertaking under grant agreement n° 115008 (NEWMEDS) of which resources are composed of EFPIA in-kind contribution and financial contribution from the European Union's Seventh Framework Programme (FP7/2007–2013). The Behavioural and Clinical Neuroscience Institute is co-funded by the Medical Research Council and the Wellcome Trust. Researchers interested in obtaining code or raw data are invited to contact us directly. The authors would like to thank Dr. Francois Gastambide at Eli Lilly for providing modafinil, Angela J Burden at Yale for help with data analyses, and Edmund Bradbury and Maarten van Dijk for their skillful technical assistance. The authors would also like to thank Begoña Gamallo Lana for helpful input on the manuscript.

Notes

Conflict of interest

L.M.S. and T.J.B. consult for Campden Instruments, Ltd. T.W.R. discloses consultancy with Cambridge Cognition, Lilly, Unilever, Lundbeck and Mundipharma, and research grants with, Lundbeck and Shionogi. M.D. and V.N. employees and shareholder of H. Lundbeck A/S. K.F. and J.N. are employees of H. Lundbeck A/S.

Footnotes

Publisher's note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Joint senior authors: Timothy J. Bussey, Adam C. Mar.

Electronic supplementary material

Supplementary Information accompanies this paper at (10.1038/s41398-018-0295-3).

References

1. Schneider M, et al. Psychiatric disorders from childhood to adulthood in 22q11.2 deletion syndrome: Results from the international consortium on brain and behavior in 22q11.2 deletion syndrome. Am. J. Psychiatry. 2014;171:627–639. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2013.13070864. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

2. Gothelf D, et al. Genetic, developmental, and physical factors associated with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in patients with velocardiofacial syndrome. Am. J. Med Genet. 2004;126B:116–121. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.20144. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

3. Niklasson L, Rasmussen P, Oskarsdottir S, Gillberg C. Neuropsychiatric disorders in the 22q11 deletion syndrome. Genet Med. 2001;3:79–84. doi: 10.1097/00125817-200101000-00017. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

4. Philip N, Bassett A. Cognitive, behavioural and psychiatric phenotype in 22q11.2 deletion syndrome. Behav. Genet. 2011;41:403–412. doi: 10.1007/s10519-011-9468-z. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

5. Robbins T. The 5-choice serial reaction time task: behavioural pharmacology and functional neurochemistry. Psychopharmacology. 2002;163:362–380. doi: 10.1007/s00213-002-1154-7. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

6. Seidman LJ, et al. Factor structure and heritability of endophenotypes in schizophrenia: findings from the Consortium on the Genetics of Schizophrenia (COGS-1) Schizophr. Res. 2015;163:73–79. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2015.01.027. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

7. Hooper SR, et al. A longitudinal examination of the psychoeducational, neurocognitive, and psychiatric functioning in children with 22q11.2 deletion syndrome. Res Dev. Disabil. 2013;34:1758–1769. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2012.12.003. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

8. Filbey FM, et al. Selective attention deficits reflect increased genetic vulnerability to schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 2008;101:169–175. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2008.01.019. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

9. Lewandowski KE, Shashi V, Berry PM, Kwapil TR. Schizophrenic-like neurocognitive deficits in children and adolescents with 22q11 deletion syndrome. Am. J. Med Genet. 2006;144B:27–36. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30379. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

10. Shashi V, et al. Evidence of gray matter reduction and dysfunction in chromosome 22q11.2 deletion syndrome. Psychiatry Res Neuroimaging. 2010;181:1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2009.07.003. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

11. Antshel KM, et al. Cognitive and psychiatric predictors to psychosis in velocardiofacial syndrome: a 3-year follow-up study. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry. 2010;49:333–344. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

12. Shashi V, et al. Altered development of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in chromosome 22q11.2 deletion syndrome: an in vivo proton spectroscopy study. Biol. Psychiat. 2012;72:684–691. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.04.023. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

13. Green MF. What are the functional consequences of neurocognitive deficits in schizophrenia? Am. J. Psychiatry. 1996;153:321–330. doi: 10.1176/ajp.153.3.321. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

14. Nuechterlein KH, et al. Attention/vigilance in schizophrenia: Performance results from a large multi-site study of the Consortium on the Genetics of Schizophrenia (COGS) Schizophr. Res. 2015;163:38–46. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2015.01.017. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

15. Lindsay EA, et al. Congenital heart disease in mice deficient for the DiGeorge syndrome region. Nature. 1999;401:379–383. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

16. Merscher S, et al. TBX1 is responsible for cardiovascular defects in Velo-Cardio-Facial/DiGeorge syndrome. Cell. 2001;104:619–629. doi: 10.1016/S0092-8674(01)00247-1. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

17. Puech A, et al. Normal cardiovascular development in mice deficient for 16 genes in 550 kb of the velocardiofacial/DiGeorge syndrome region. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2000;97:10090–10095. doi: 10.1073/pnas.97.18.10090. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

18. Kimber WL, et al. Deletion of 150 kb in the minimal DiGeorge/velocardiofacial syndrome critical region in mouse. Human. Mol. Genet. 1999;8:2229–2237. doi: 10.1093/hmg/8.12.2229. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

19. Stark KL, et al. Altered brain microRNA biogenesis contributes to phenotypic deficits in a 22q11-deletion mouse model. Nat. Genet. 2008;40:751–760. doi: 10.1038/ng.138. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

20. Sigurdsson T, Stark KL, Karayiorgou M, Gogos JA, Gordon JA. Impaired hippocampal-prefrontal synchrony in a genetic mouse model of schizophrenia. Nature. 2010;464:763–767. doi: 10.1038/nature08855. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

21. Earls LR, et al. Age-dependent microRNA control of synaptic plasticity in 22q11 deletion syndrome and schizophrenia. J. Neurosci. 2012;32:14132–14144. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1312-12.2012. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

22. Meechan DW, et al. Cognitive ability is associated with altered medial frontal cortical circuits in the LgDel mouse model of 22q11.2DS. Cereb. Cortex. 2015;25:1143–1151. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht308. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

23. Nilsson SR, et al. Assessing the cognitive translational potential of a mouse model of the 22q11.2 microdeletion syndrome. Cereb. Cortex. 2016;26:3991–4003. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhw229. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

24. Tamura M, Mukai J, Gordon JA, Gogos JA. Developmental inhibition of Gsk3 rescues behavioral and neurophysiological deficits in a mouse model of schizophrenia predisposition. Neuron. 2016;89:1100–1109. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.01.025. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

25. Diamantopoulou A, et al. Loss-of-function mutation in Mirta22/Emc10rescues specific schizophrenia-related phenotypes in a mouse model of the 22q11.2 deletion. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2017;114:E6127–E6136. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1615719114. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

26. Jones MW, Wilson MA. Theta rhythms coordinate hippocampal–prefrontal interactions in a spatial memory task. PLoS Biol. 2005;3:e402–e413. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0030402. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

27. Benchenane K, Tiesinga PH, Battaglia FP. Oscillations in the prefrontal cortex: a gateway to memory and attention. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2011;21:475–485. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2011.01.004. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

28. Hyman JM, Hasselmo ME, Seamans JK. What is the functional relevance of prefrontal cortex entrainment to hippocampal theta rhythms? Front Neurosci. 2011;5:24. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2011.00024. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

29. Karayiorgou M, Simon TJ, Gogos JA. 22q11.2 microdeletions: linking DNA structural variation to brain dysfunction and schizophrenia. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2010;11:402–416. doi: 10.1038/nrn2841. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

30. Kim CH, et al. The continuous performance test (rCPT) for mice: a novel operant touchscreen test of attentional function. Psychopharmacology. 2015;232:3947–3966. doi: 10.1007/s00213-015-4081-0. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

31. Mar AC, et al. MAM-E17 rat model impairments on a novel continuous performance task: effects of potential cognitive enhancing drugs. Psychopharmacology. 2017;234:2837–2857. doi: 10.1007/s00213-017-4679-5. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

32. Bähner F, Meyer-Lindenberg A. Hippocampal–prefrontal connectivity as a translational phenotype for schizophrenia. Eur. Neuropsychopharm. 2017;27:93–106. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2016.12.007. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

33. Berridge CW, Arnsten AFT. Psychostimulants and motivated behavior: Arousal and cognition. Neurosci. Biobehav Rev. 2013;37:1976–1984. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.11.005. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

34. Scoriels L, Barnett JH, Soma PK, Sahakian BJ, Jones PB. Effects of modafinil on cognitive functions in first episode psychosis. Psychopharmacology. 2011;220:249–258. doi: 10.1007/s00213-011-2472-4. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

35. Chudasama Y. Animal models of prefrontal-executive function. Behav. Neurosci. 2011;125:327–343. doi: 10.1037/a0023766. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

36. Hvoslef-Eide M, et al. The NEWMEDS rodent touchscreen test battery for cognition relevant to schizophrenia. Psychopharmacology. 2015;232:3853–3872. doi: 10.1007/s00213-015-4007-x. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

37. Green T, et al. The effect of methylphenidate on prefrontal cognitive functioning, inattention, and hyperactivity in velocardiofacial syndrome. J. Child Adolesc. Psychopharmacol. 2011;21:589–595. doi: 10.1089/cap.2011.0042. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

38. Gothelf D, et al. Methylphenidate treatment for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in children and adolescents with velocardiofacial syndrome: an open-label study. J. Clin. Psychiatry. 2003;64:1163–1169. doi: 10.4088/JCP.v64n1004. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

39. Didriksen M, et al. Persistent gating deficit and increased sensitivity to NMDA receptor antagonism after puberty in a new mouse model of the human 22q11.2 microdeletion syndrome: a study in male mice. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. 2017;42:48–58. doi: 10.1503/jpn.150381. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

40. Mar AC, et al. The touchscreen operant platform for assessing executive function in rats and mice. Nat. Protoc. 2013;8:1985–2005. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2013.123. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

41. Heath CJ, Bussey TJ, Saksida LM. Motivational assessment of mice using the touchscreen operant testing system: effects of dopaminergic drugs. Psychopharmacology. 2015;232:4043–4057. doi: 10.1007/s00213-015-4009-8. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

42. Tomlinson A, et al. Pay attention to impulsivity: modelling low attentive and high impulsive subtypes of adult ADHD in the 5-choice continuous performance task (5C-CPT) in female rats. Eur. Neuropsychopharm. 2014;24:1371–1380. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2014.04.008. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

43. Eagle DM, Tufft MRA, Goodchild HL, Robbins TW. Differential effects of modafinil and methylphenidate on stop-signal reaction time task performance in the rat, and interactions with the dopamine receptor antagonist cis-flupenthixol. Psychopharmacology. 2007;192:193–206. doi: 10.1007/s00213-007-0701-7. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

44. Takillah S, et al. Acute stress affects the expression of hippocampal mu oscillations in an age-dependent manner. Front Aging Neurosci. 2017;9:295. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2017.00295. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

45. Vanderwolf CH. Hippocampal electrical activity and voluntary movement in the rat. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 1969;26:407–418. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(69)90092-3. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

46. Bokil H, Andrews P, Kulkarni JE, Mehta S, Mitra PP. Chronux: a platform for analyzing neural signals. J. Neurosci. Methods. 2010;192:146–151. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2010.06.020. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

47. Swets JA. Measuring the Accuracy of Diagnostic Systems. Science. 1988;240:1285–1293. doi: 10.1126/science.3287615. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

48. Macmillan N. A., Creelman C. D. Detection Theory: a User's Guide. 2nd ed. Lawrence Erlbaum, 2004.

49. Niarchou Maria, Calkins Monica E, Moore Tyler M, Tang Sunny X, McDonald-McGinn Donna M, Zackai Elaine H, Emanuel Beverly S, Gur Ruben C, Gur Raquel E. Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder Symptoms and Psychosis in 22q11.2 Deletion Syndrome. Schizophrenia Bulletin. 2017;44(4):824–833. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbx113. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

50. Schoch K, et al. Applicability of the nonverbal learning disability paradigm for children with 22q11.2 deletion syndrome. J. Learn Disabil. 2012;47:153–166. doi: 10.1177/0022219412443556. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

51. Woods SP, Lovejoy DW, Ball JD. Neuropsychological characteristics of adults with ADHD: a comprehensive review of initial studies. Clin. Neuropsychol. 2002;16:12–34. doi: 10.1076/clin.16.1.12.8336. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

52. Cornblatt BA, Keilp JG. Impaired attention, genetics, and the pathophysiology of schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 1994;20:31–46. doi: 10.1093/schbul/20.1.31. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

53. Nuechterlein KH, et al. Developmental processes in schizophrenic disorders: longitudinal studies of vulnerability and stress. Schizophr. Bull. 1992;18:387–425. doi: 10.1093/schbul/18.3.387. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

54. Vaughn AJ, et al. Relation between outcomes on a continuous performance test and ADHD symptoms over time. J. Abnorm Child Psychol. 2011;39:853–864. doi: 10.1007/s10802-011-9501-y. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

55. Sobin C, et al. Networks of attention in children with the 22q11 deletion syndrome. Dev. Neuropsychol. 2004;26:611–626. doi: 10.1207/s15326942dn2602_5. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

56. Cornblatt BA, Malhotra AK. Impaired attention as an endophenotype for molecular genetic studies of schizophrenia. Am. J. Med Genet. 2001;105:11–15. doi: 10.1002/1096-8628(20010108)105:1<11::AID-AJMG1045>3.0.CO;2-G. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

57. Niarchou M, et al. Psychopathology and cognition in children with 22q11.2 deletion syndrome. Br. J. Psychiatry. 2014;204:46–54. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.113.132324. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

58. Beaton EA, Simon TJ. How might stress contribute to increased risk for schizophrenia in children with chromosome 22q11.2 deletion syndrome? J. Neurodev. Disord. 2010;3:68–75. doi: 10.1007/s11689-010-9069-9. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

59. Ayhan Y, McFarland R, Pletnikov MV. Animal models of gene–environment interaction in schizophrenia: a dimensional perspective. Prog. Neurobiol. 2016;136:1–27. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2015.10.002. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

60. Eliez S, Schmitt JE, White CD, Reiss AL. Children and adolescents with velocardiofacial syndrome: a volumetric MRI study. Am. J. Psychiatry. 2000;157:409–415. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.157.3.409. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

61. Barnea-Goraly N, et al. Investigation of white matter structure in velocardiofacial syndrome: A diffusion tensor imaging study. Am. J. Psychiatry. 2003;160:1863–1869. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.10.1863. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

62. Ottet MC, et al. Reduced fronto-temporal and limbic connectivity in the 22q11.2 deletion syndrome: Vulnerability markers for developing schizophrenia? PLoS One. 2013;8:e58429–8. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0058429. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

63. Ellegood J, et al. Neuroanatomical phenotypes in a mouse model of the 22q11.2 microdeletion. Mol. Psychiatry. 2014;19:99–107. doi: 10.1038/mp.2013.112. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

64. Xu Bin, et al. of a neuronal inhibitor due to mirna dysregulation in a schizophrenia-related microdeletion. Cell. 2013;152:262–275. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2012.11.052. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

65. Fenelon K, et al. The pattern of cortical dysfunction in a mouse model of a schizophrenia-related microdeletion. J. Neurosci. 2013;33:14825–14839. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1611-13.2013. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

66. Robbins TW, Arnsten AFT. The neuropsychopharmacology of fronto-executive function: Monoaminergic modulation. Annu Rev. Neurosci. 2009;32:267–287. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.051508.135535. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

67. Lindenmayer JP, Nasrallah H, Pucci M, James S, Citrome L. A systematic review of psychostimulant treatment of negative symptoms of schizophrenia: Challenges and therapeutic opportunities. Schizophr. Res. 2013;147:241–252. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2013.03.019. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

68. Scoriels L, Jones PB, Sahakian BJ. Modafinil effects on cognition and emotion in schizophrenia and its neurochemical modulation in the brain. Neuropharmacology. 2013;64:168–184. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2012.07.011. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

69. Bizarro L, Patel S, Murtagh C, Stolerman IP. Differential effects of psychomotor stimulants on attentional performance in rats: nicotine, amphetamine, caffeine and methylphenidate. Behav. Pharmacol. 2004;15:195–206. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

70. Grottick A, Higgins G. Assessing a vigilance decrement in aged rats: effects of pre-feeding, task manipulation, and psychostimulants. Psychopharmacology. 2002;164:33–41. doi: 10.1007/s00213-002-1174-3. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

71. Morgan RE, Crowley JM, Smith RH, LaRoche RB, Dopheide MM. Modafinil improves attention, inhibitory control, and reaction time in healthy, middle-aged rats. Pharmacol. Biochem Behav. 2007;86:531–541. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2007.01.015. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

72. Caballero-Puntiverio M, Fitzpatrick CM, Woldbye DP, Andreasen JT. Effects of amphetamine and methylphenidate on attentional performance and impulsivity in the mouse 5-Choice Serial Reaction Time Task. J. Psychopharmacol. 2017;31:272–283. doi: 10.1177/0269881116684339. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

73. Liu YP, Tung CS, Lin YL, Chuang CH. Wake-promoting agent modafinil worsened attentional performance following REM sleep deprivation in a young-adult rat model of 5-choice serial reaction time task. Psychopharmacology. 2010;213:155–166. doi: 10.1007/s00213-010-2019-0. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

74. Waters KA, Burnham KE, O'Connor D, Dawson GR, Dias R. Assessment of modafinil on attentional processes in a five-choice serial reaction time test in the rat. J. Psychopharmacol. 2016;19:149–158. doi: 10.1177/0269881105048995. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

75. Turner DC, et al. Modafinil improves cognition and attentional set shifting in patients with chronic schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharm. 2004;29:1363–1373. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300457. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

76. Marchant NL, et al. Modafinil improves rapid shifts of attention. Psychopharmacology. 2008;202:487–495. doi: 10.1007/s00213-008-1395-1. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

77. Turner DC, et al. Cognitive enhancing effects of modafinil in healthy volunteers. Psychopharmacology. 2003;165:260–269. doi: 10.1007/s00213-002-1250-8. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

78. Minzenberg MJ, Carter CS. Modafinil: a review of neurochemical actions and effects on cognition. Neuropsychopharm. 2007;33:1477–1502. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301534. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

79. Ferraro L, et al. Amplification of cortical serotonin release: a further neurochemical action of the vigilance-promoting drug modafinil. Neuropharmacology. 2000;39:1974–1983. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3908(00)00019-8. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

80. Oades RD. Differential measures of 'sustained attention' in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity or tic disorders: relations to monoamine metabolism. Psychiatry Res. 2000;93:165–178. doi: 10.1016/S0165-1781(00)00105-0. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

81. Groman SM, et al. Monoamine levels within the orbitofrontal cortex and putamen interact to predict reversal learning performance. Biol. Psychiat. 2013;73:756–762. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.12.002. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

82. Rubia K, et al. Methylphenidate normalises activation and functional connectivity deficits in attention and motivation networks in medication-naIve children with ADHD during a rewarded continuous performance task. Neuropharmacology. 2009;57:640–652. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2009.08.013. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

83. Pietrzak RH, Snyder PJ, Maruff P. Use of an acute challenge with d-amphetamine to model cognitive improvement in chronic schizophrenia. Hum. Psychopharmacol. Clin. Exp. 2010;25:353–358. doi: 10.1002/hup.1118. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

84. Cesarec Z, Nyman AK. Differential response to amphetamine in schizophrenia. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 1985;71:523–538. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1985.tb05066.x. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

85. Andrzejewski ME, et al. The effects of clinically relevant doses of amphetamine and methylphenidate on signal detection and DRL in rats. Neuropharmacology. 2014;79:634–641. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2014.01.018. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

86. Riccio CA, Waldrop J, Reynolds CR, Lowe P. Effects of stimulants on the continuous performance test (CPT): Implications for CPT use and interpretation. J. Neuropsychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 2001;13:326–335. doi: 10.1176/jnp.13.3.326. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

87. Spencer RC, Devilbiss DM, Berridge CW. The Cognition-Enhancing Effects of Psychostimulants Involve Direct Action in the Prefrontal Cortex. Biol. Psychiat. 2015;77:940–950. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.09.013. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

88. van Gaalen MM, Brueggeman RJ, Bronius PFC, Schoffelmeer ANM. Vanderschuren LJMJ. Behavioral disinhibition requires dopamine receptor activation. Psychopharmacology. 2006;187:73–85. doi: 10.1007/s00213-006-0396-1. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

89. Cole BJ, Robbins TW. Amphetamine impairs the discriminative performance of rats with dorsal noradrenergic bundle lesions on a 5-choice serial reaction-time-task: New evidence for central dopaminergic-noradrenergic interactions. Psychopharmacology. 1987;91:458–466. doi: 10.1007/BF00216011. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

90. Matsumoto M, et al. Catechol O-methyltransferase mRNA expression in human and rat brain: evidence for a role in cortical neuronal function. Neuroscience. 2003;116:127–137. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4522(02)00556-0. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

91. During MJ, Bean AJ, Roth RH. Effects of CNS stimulants on the in vivo release of the colocalized transmitters, dopamine and neurotensin, from rat prefrontal cortex. Neurosci. Lett. 1992;140:129–133. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(92)90698-7. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

92. Hertel P, et al. Effects of D-amphetamine and phencyclidine on behavior and extracellular concentrations of neurotensin and dopamine in the ventral striatum and the medial prefrontal cortex of the rat. Behav. Brain Res. 1995;72:103–114. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(96)00138-6. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

93. Gerber DJ, et al. Hyperactivity, elevated dopaminergic transmission, and response to amphetamine in M1 muscarinic acetylcholine receptor-deficient mice. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2001;98:15312–15317. doi: 10.1073/pnas.261583798. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

94. Takahashi N, et al. VMAT2 knockout mice: Heterozygotes display reduced amphetamine-conditioned reward, enhanced amphetamine locomotion, and enhanced MPTP toxicity. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 1997;94:9938–9943. doi: 10.1073/pnas.94.18.9938. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

95. Bizarro L, Stolerman IP. Attentional effects of nicotine and amphetamine in rats at different levels of motivation. Psychopharmacology. 2003;170:271–277. doi: 10.1007/s00213-003-1543-6. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

96. Huotari M, García-Horsman JA, Karayiorgou M, Gogos JA, Männistö PT. d-Amphetamine responses in catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) disrupted mice. Psychopharmacology. 2004;172:1–10. doi: 10.1007/s00213-003-1627-3. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

97. Florin SM, Kuczenski R, Segal DS. Regional extracellular norepinephrine responses to amphetamine and cocaine and effects of clonidine pretreatment. Brain Res. 1994;654:53–62. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(94)91570-9. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

98. Berridge CW, Stalnaker TA. Relationship between low-dose amphetamine-induced arousal and extracellular norepinephrine and dopamine levels within prefrontal cortex. Synapse. 2002;46:140–149. doi: 10.1002/syn.10131. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

99. Lapish CC, Chiang J, Wang JZ, Phillips AG. Oscillatory power and synchrony in the rat forebrain are altered by a sensitizing regime of d-amphetamine. Neuroscience. 2012;203:108–121. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2011.12.020. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]


Articles from Translational Psychiatry are provided here courtesy of Nature Publishing Group


parkeryage1992.blogspot.com

Source: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6235862/

0 Response to "Gradual Onset Continuous Performance Test 100 64"

Postar um comentário

Iklan Atas Artikel

Iklan Tengah Artikel 1

Iklan Tengah Artikel 2

Iklan Bawah Artikel